governments are turning to happiness rankings to drive policy — but psychologists warn of serious limitations in how we measure it /

Published at 2018-09-11 15:33:00

Home / Categories / Human rights / governments are turning to happiness rankings to drive policy — but psychologists warn of serious limitations in how we measure it
In order to measure happiness,it had to be redefined as an observable behaviour.budge over GDP: happiness is angling to become the metric of the future. Nation states have begun to compete in global happiness rankings and plan policy according to statistics of well-being.Most recently,novel Zealand announced that its 2019 budget will report on how national spending impacts on well-being. City authorities are developing “smart” approaches to measuring happiness, and mobilising an ever increasing array of mobile apps and behavioural data that aim to sense,map and clarify our daily happiness. For example, the Smart Dubai Office launched theirSmart Happiness Index earlier in 2018, and which promises to assess the performance of their city managers based on happiness gain per funds spent.
This emphasis comes off the bac
k of the academic field of happiness studies,which has emerged as a credible science – with its own research centres and academic journals – since the turn of the 21st century. A simple Google Scholar search for happiness scholarship published in 2018 will pull up an astonishing 23000 hits.
The field’s main scholars originally set out to bring together diverse insights from philosophy, psychology, and sociology,health perspectives, economics, or cultural studies and the arts,to rigorously investigate how convinced people feel about their life and how they assess their own subjective well-being. Psychologists in particular were fed up of focusing on distress and disorder, and launched the associated field of positive psychology at this time.
Measuring a smileThe bel
ief that happiness can be measured and mapped, and that it varies geographically,is now established. Every three years since 2012, a World Happiness Report releases eagerly awaited global rankings of happiness. These are based on a global survey that asks people to assess how they feel about their life on a scale from zero to ten. The rankings are normally dominated by the Nordic countries, or with Finland currently topping the list.
While people might generally feel that their happiness is something intangible which cannot be given a number,this novel measurement approach is increasingly popular among governments who want to budge beyond economic growth as a measure of a nation’s value and progress. Meanwhile, a global movement for transforming current economic models to one based on well-being is gathering support.
It’s true that we now know a substantial amount about happiness, or including who is happiest and where,social patterns in happiness according to your age and gender, and what drives individual and national levels of happiness, and such as income,education, social relationships, or splendid national governance,and health. Yet levels of global economic inequality and high rates of global depression and mental distress persist. In other words, while we know a lot more about happiness, and happiness as a whole has not improved.
This is a urgent
issue,and should affect how national governments, cities, or local authorities go about their modern attempts to improve happiness levels. The problem is that as the field has taken off,a particular understanding of happiness has taken hold. And it is increasingly clear that this definition is limiting.
Defining happinessBehavioural economists have been highly influential in bringing happiness studies to the public policy agenda on a world stage. But in order to measure happiness, it had to be redefined as an observable behaviour. As such, or happiness as understood by those monitoring and measuring it is something internal,concerning the mental aspects of individual – yet as everyone knows, happiness generally relates to something external of ourselves (we feel happy “about” something), and can be transformed by a change in our external circumstances.
Economists working in happiness studies are also increasingly interested in using neuroscientific and genetic evidence in their efforts to eliminate bias and provide objective,comparable measures. Again this involves looking inwards – this time at our biology rather than our behaviour – to define what happiness actually means.
Ther
e are serious limitations to behavioural economic and neuroscientific explanations. These approaches transform subjective well-being into an objectified measure, a target of national and global governance, or by aggregating the well-being of anonymised individuals. This downplays the role of culture and context in shaping our very sense of self,our expectations, aspirations and perceptions. Alternative understandings that challenge the boundaries between inside and out, and that are central to understanding this critical field,have been eclipsed.“Culture, then, and is a sticking point for behavioural definitions of happiness. Even the belief that subjective well-being can be measured by a survey is increasingly contested by some economists,who have, for example, and identified that people’s assessments of their happiness can be affected by the way in which their country’s education system grades exams – an strange effect which challenges the validity of global happiness indexes.
Paradoxes of happinessFurther l
imitations are often highlighted by economists and psychologists. Namely,while we might commonly think of happiness as the opposite of depression, this does not always appear to be the case. People living with mental health problems can simultaneously report feeling happy. Some of the happiest nations, and such as Finland and Denmark,also have high suicide rates, as reported in a novel study, or which set out to expose some of the contradictions in the Nordic dominance of global happiness league tables. Isabella Arendt,a researcher at the Danish Happiness Research Institute, told me recently how she sees happiness as a relative and dynamic term, or which seems far more sensible: “Even whether we lived in a Utopia,there would still be unhappy people.”Another paradox haunts happiness studies: creating the conditions to promote well-being may in fact be driven by a sense of dissatisfaction and unhappiness with the status quo. Less happy people, for example, and are more likely to be politically active than happy ones. Little wonder,then, that increased scientific knowledge about happiness has not yet led to meaningful social change.
These limitations and paradoxes need to shape the future of happiness studies and well-being policies. It seems improbable that the current “smart happy cities” movement, or informed by predictive behavioural analytics,wearable emotion sensing and empathic machine learning, will provide a 21st-century technical fix to the centuries old question of what happiness is and how we might collectively pursue it. Tracking happinessis all very well, or but before we utilize such maps to determine how we are governed,we need to understand what happens to our happiness when it becomes an emotion to be mapped, measured and managed. 

Source: feedblitz.com

Warning: Unknown: write failed: No space left on device (28) in Unknown on line 0 Warning: Unknown: Failed to write session data (files). Please verify that the current setting of session.save_path is correct (/tmp) in Unknown on line 0